Motor milestones mark exciting transitions in our children’s lives, but there is no single, universal schedule that all children follow. For example, studies indicate that more than 50% of infants can
Yet the timing and expression of motor development varies substantially from baby to baby – and from one culture to the next. Although it’s less common, there are babies who have learned to roll over when they are only 2 months old, and babies who stand (unassisted) when they are less than 8 months old (Nelson et al 2004). There are societies where infants never learn to crawl, and societies where children don’t normally begin walking until well past 18 months (Rachwani et al 2020; Kaplan and Dove 1987).
What’s going on here? We can definitely rule out the idea that all healthy babies develop at the same rate. Clearly, that isn’t happening. We can also reject the notion that individual children are biologically programmed to hit their motor milestones at specific, predetermined ages. As we’ll see below, there is strong evidence showing that motor development is influenced by parenting practices and other environmental factors: Children develop skills faster when they get lots of opportunities to practice.
So what does this mean if we want to know what to expect, and find out if our own children are on track?
Basically, it means that we need to think about motor milestones as likely to occur within certain time windows – sometimes relatively broad time windows. Individuals develop at their own pace, depending on their personal characteristics and experiences.
In this article, we will go over the development time windows during which most children achieve major motor milestones – both gross motor milestones, and fine motor milestones. Then we’ll talk about when to be concerned about the possibility of a motor delay. I’ll include an age-by-age timeline of when to talk to your pediatrician if your baby seems to be lagging, based on the latest, expert recommendations. Finally, we’ll talk about the reasons that individual children vary in the timing of milestones, and I’ll present some evidence-based tips for supporting the development of motor skills.
Gross motor milestones depend on the coordination of large muscles in the legs, trunk, and arms. They include modes of locomotion, like crawling, walking, and running. But they also include motor skills related to posture, like being able to hold your head erect, sitting up without support, and standing.
What are the major gross motor milestones – the milestones that parents are supposed to notice and track? Historically, pediatricians and developmental scientists have emphasized these postural and locomotory milestones for infancy:
When should we expect children to achieve gross motor milestones?
Individual variation makes this tricky, even if we restrict ourselves to children living in the same culture. For instance, take the first milestone – a baby lifting his or her head while lying prone (belly down). A study of Canadian babies suggests that around 5% of children can achieve this shortly after birth. Half can do it before they are 1.5 months old, and 95% will have reached this milestone by the time they are 4 months old (Rachwani et al 2020; Piper and Darrah 1994).
We could try to mash this up with a single number – an average – but that wouldn’t be very helpful for appreciating the range of what’s normal. A better way to represent the development of motor skills is to think in terms of developmental windows – time periods during which the vast majority of children achieve a given milestones.
So we can characterize the milestone, “Lifts head when lying on belly,” with a time window based on the statistics from the Canadian study. For children encountering conditions similar to those in Canada, we may expect that about 95% of children will achieve this motor milestone sometime between birth and 4 months (Piper and Darrah 1994).
Similarly, we can use the same study to come up with estimated time windows for
If you prefer a more visual approach, here’s an infographic I made to illustrate, adapted from a figure published by the World Health Organization (WHO 2006b). It shows the time windows during which approximately 98% of infants reach the remaining 6 gross motor milestones mentioned above.
As you can see, some gross motor milestones tend to occur earlier than others, but the windows are wide, and they overlap each other. The resulting picture doesn’t predict when your baby will hit any particular milestone – not in any fine-grained sense. But it provides us with a realistic time range.
Two-month-old babies can hold onto small objects – if we place these objects directly into their hands. And they are likely to bring the items up to their mouths to investigate (Rochat 1989). But the grasp of a young infant isn’t very secure or reliable. When babies’ arms flail around, they are likely to lose their grip on whatever they are holding.
Between the ages of 4 and 6 months, most babies will have developed the manual dexterity to hold onto and shake a toy. They are also developing the ability to move an object back and forth between hands.
Babies can successfully reach for a stationary object, but their movements are jerky, and babies aren’t yet good at catching a moving object. Babies don’t yet understand how to grasp large objects efficiently – they don’t show a preference for doing it with both hands.
Between 6 and 9 months, these skills improve considerably. Babies become proficient at catching hold of moving objects. For instance, they can grab rolling balls, and judge when some balls are rolling too fast to catch (van Hof et al 2008). By 11 months, babies also show better planning for picking up large objects – they consistently reach with both hands at once (Fagard and Jacquet 1996).
By 8 to 10 months, most babies are showing off a number of fine motor skills. They are developing the ability to grip small objects between the thumb and index finger – the so-called “pincer grasp”. Many babies can drink from a cup (albeit with help!), and they are figuring out how to eat with a spoon.
But their attempts are awkward. For instance, if you provide them with a loaded spoon, they are likely to pick it up by the bowl end – not the handle (McCarty et al 2001; van Roon et al 2003). Moreover, they will hold onto the spoon with a fist grip, not a precision (thumb-to-index figure) grip. By 14 months babies are more adept. They might still hold the spoon in a fist grip, but they’ve learned how to hold it by the handle (van Roon et al 2003).
Around this same time – from approximately 12 months onward – many babies can use writing implements to draw random-looking marks and dots. By 18 months, these efforts may become more controlled and organized, and may include straight lines and zig-zags (Dunst and Gorman 2009). More complex drawings – of geometric shapes, and figures with identifiable features (like a blob creature with legs) – develop slowly, and may not appear until a child is three years old (Dunst and Gorman 2009).
And then there are the fine motor skills that involve placing or stacking objects. In a study tracking the fine motor development of 37 infants in the United States, researchers found that more than 85% of the children had begun stacking at least one block on top of another between the ages of 10 and 14 months. By 18 months, about 40% of the children could create block towers composed of 3 elements or more. By 24 months, more than 85% of the kids had reached this fine motor milestone (Marcinowski et al 2019).
First, you need to trust your intuitions. If something seems wrong or worrying, talk to your pediatrician. If it turns out that your baby is having problems, early intervention can make a big difference.
But what if you’re merely noticing that your baby is taking longer than most to reach a particular motor milestone. At what point should your baby should be screened for a possible developmental delay?
In the absence of any other signs or concerns, one rule of thumb is to pay attention to the 90th percentile – the age by which 90% of children have achieved a given milestone (Sices 2007). Another approach is to set the bar lower, at the age when a milestone has been reached 75% of the population (Zubler et al 2022).
Either way, the idea is to compare children, and then focus on individuals who are slower than a selected percentage of their peers. If a child still hasn’t reached a milestone by the specified age, it doesn’t mean that child is especially likely to have a developmental problem. But it’s an indication to move forward with screening. As noted, early intervention can be important, so it makes sense to take a proactive approach.
Here are some of the latest, evidence-informed milestone recommendations, as proposed by researchers working for the American Academy of Pediatrics and the Center for Disease Control (Zubler et al 2022). Note that the ages represent the cut-off dates for observing the given milestones. If your child has not reached a milestone by the listed date, that’s reason to consult with your pediatrician.
2 months
4 months
6 months
9 months
12 months
15 months
18 months
24 months
30 months
3 years
4 years
5 years
Not necessarily. Babies don’t always hit these milestones in the same order, and one of the milestones – crawling – isn’t even universal.
If you look at the time window graphic, you might reasonably assume that your baby will hit gross motor milestones in the following sequence:
(1) sitting up without support; (2) crawling on hands and knees; (3) standing with assistance; (4) walking with assistance; (5) standing without support; and (6) walking without support.
And indeed, when the World Health Organization (WHO) tracked the development of babies in 5 countries (Ghana, India, Norway, Oman and the USA), this pattern was found in the largest percentage of infants – about 42% of them.
But more than a third of the babies achieved milestone #3 (standing with assistance) before they crawled. Almost 9% of the babies also hit milestone #4 (walking with assistance) before crawling.
Another 10% of babies mixed the order up in even more exotic ways, and approximately 4% of babies never crawled on their hands and knees (WHO 2006a).
Other studies have reported even higher rates of babies who never crawled — babies who were healthy and went on to walk within the normal time window.
So there isn’t a master sequence of motor development milestones that all babies follow. As motor development experts Karen Adolph and John Franchak (2016) explain:
“The milestone charts suggest an orderly, age-related march through a series of stages, but developmental pathways can differ and individual infants do not strictly adhere to the normative sequence derived from average onset ages. Infants can acquire skills in various orders, skip stages, and revert to earlier forms.”
Some of it is cultural.
For example, in some African countries, parents actively train their babies to sit, stand, and walk. They provide infants with lots of practice, and this appears to accelerate the development of upright posture (Super 1976; Bril and Sabatier 1986; Karasik et al 2015; Adolph and Robinson 2015).
The notion is supported by experimental work (Zelazo 1983). Newborn babies have a “stepping reflex”: If you hold a baby so that soles of his feet brush against the ground, the baby will spontaneously take steps — long before the baby is capable of standing under his own weight. The reflex usually disappears over time, but not if babies are given daily opportunities to practice the action, and such babies have reached the milestone of walking (without assistance) at an earlier age. (You can read more about learning to walk in my article, “When do babies start walking, and how does it develop?”)
So we’ve got evidence that parenting practices can speed up the pace of motor development. And the converse is also true: Parenting can slow it down.
In places where parents adopt a hands-off approach – or actively prevent babies from moving around during the day – infants take longer to achieve certain motor milestones (WHO 2006b; Mei 1994; Adolph and Robinson 2015; Adolf et al 2018).
What about crawling? Environmental factors play a big role there, too.
In the contemporary United States, parents expect babies to crawl, and they provide them with opportunities to do so. But this isn’t true everywhere, and it probably wasn’t true for our hunter-gatherer ancestors.
Crawling outdoors – in a world inhabited by predators – wouldn’t have been a good idea, and indeed, contemporary hunter-gatherers don’t encourage their infants to crawl.
As I explain in this guide to the development of crawling, it’s not unusual for babies to reject hands-and-knees crawling in favor of other methods of getting around – like scooting around on their bottoms, or rolling from place to place.
And it’s clear that motor milestones are influenced by genetics.
When researchers have controlled for the effects of culture and parenting, they’ve found that genetic factors have an important impact on the timing of motor milestones (Smith et al 2017).
Siblings don’t reach motor milestones at exactly the same time, even if they are raised under similar conditions. Individual differences in temperament, body fat composition, and other characteristics — characteristics influenced by genes — can affect a child’s activity patterns, leading some individuals to spend more time practicing developing motor skills.
Give your baby lots of “tummy time.”
As I note in this article, it’s clear that “tummy time” is important. Babies develop better muscle control when they spend supervised time on their stomachs. It’s good for building neck strength, and it helps babies develop the ability to roll, crawl, and sit up from a lying position (Kuo et al 2008).
Help babies practice an upright posture.
We’ve also seen how parents can support the development of sitting and standing. Practice sessions – where you help your baby adopt an upright posture by providing support with your hands – may speed up development.
Help babies reach and grasp.
Not surprisingly, babies learn faster when we provide them with opportunities to touch, hold, and reach for objects.
For example, in experiments using mittens and toys covered in Velcro®, babies as young as 3 months have gotten extra practice handling objects that would ordinarily be hard to grasp. When parents encourage their babies to explore objects with such “sticky mittens,” babies have shown long-term developmental benefits (Needham et al 2017; Libertus et al 2015).
Let babies bang.
It’s noisy and obnoxious, but researchers think that babies develop important motor skills when they grab onto an object and bang away (Kahrs et al 2012). Just make sure the object is safe for your baby to use!
Encourage free play – and make yourself a visible, responsive, and non-bossy playmate.
Babies exercise more – and spend more time interacting with objects – when we provide them with the time and space to engage in free play (Adolf and Koch 2019). And babies benefit when we get down on the floor to interact with them.
For more information about baby development, see this index to Parenting Science articles.
Adolph K. 2008. Motor and physical development: Locomotion. In M.M. Haith and J.B. Benson (Eds), Encyclopedia of infant and early childhood development, M.M. Haith and J.B. Benson (Eds), San Diego: Academic Press, pp. 359-373.
Adolph KE and Robinson SR 2015. Motor development. In R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.) and L. Liben and U. Muller (Eds), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: Vol. 2: Cognitive processes (7th ed.) New York: Wiley, pp. 114-157
Adolph KE and Hoch JE 2019. Motor development: Embodied, embedded, enculturated, and enabling. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 141-164.
Adolph KE, Hoch JE, Cole WG. 2018. Development (of Walking): 15 Suggestions. Trends Cogn Sci. 22(8):699-711.
Bril B and Sabatier C. The cultural context of motor development: Postural manipulations in the daily life of Bambara babies (Mali) International Journal of Behavioral Development. 1986;9:439–453.
Dunst C., Gorman E. (2009). Development of infant and toddler mark making and scribbling. Cent. Early Learn. Lit. Rev. 2, 1–16.
Fagard J, Jacquet AY. Changes in reaching and grasping objects of different sizes between 7 and 13 months of age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 1996;14:65–78.
Kahrs BA, Jung WP, Lockman JJ. 2012. What is the role of infant banging in the development of tool use? Exp Brain Res. 218(2):315-20.
Kaplan H and Dove H. 1987. Infant development among the Ache of eastern Paraguay. Developmental Psychology, 23(2): 190–198.
Kuo YL, Liao HF, Chen PC, Hsieh WS, Hwang AW. 2008. The influence of wakeful prone positioning on motor development during the early life. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 29(5):367-76.
Libertus K, Joh AS, Needham AW. 2016. Motor training at 3 months affects object exploration 12 months later. Dev Sci. 19(6):1058-1066.
Marcinowski EC, Nelson E, Campbell JM, Michel GF. 2019. The Development of Object Construction from Infancy through Toddlerhood. Infancy. 24(3):368-391.
McCarty ME, Clifton RK, and Collard RR. 2001. The beginnings of tool use by infants and toddlers. Infancy 2: 233-56.
Mei, J. 1994. The Northern Chinese custom of rearing babies in sandbags: implications for motor and intellectual development. In: vanRossum, J.; Laszlo, J., editors. Motor development: Aspects of normal and delayed development. Amsterdam: VU Uitgeverij.
Needham AW, Wiesen SE, Hejazi JN, Libertus K, Christopher C. 2017. Characteristics of brief sticky mittens training that lead to increases in object exploration. J Exp Child Psychol. 164:209-224
Nelson EA1, Yu LM, Wong D, Wong HY, Yim L. 2004. Rolling over in infants: age, ethnicity, and cultural differences. Dev Med Child Neurol. 46(10):706-9.
Rachwani J, Hoch J, and Adolph KE. 2020. Action in development: Plasticity, variability, and flexibility. In J. J. Lockman and C. S. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of infant development: Brain, behavior, and cultural context (pp. 469–494). Cambridge University Press.
Rochat R 1989 Object Manipulation and Exploration in 2- to 5-Month-Old Infants Developmental Psychology 25 (6): 871-884
Sices L. 2007. Use of developmental milestones in pediatric residency training and practice: time to rethink the meaning of the mean. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 28(1):47-52.
Smith L, van Jaarsveld CHM, Llewellyn CH, Fildes A, López Sánchez GF, Wardle J, Fisher A. 2017. Genetic and Environmental Influences on Developmental Milestones and Movement: Results From the Gemini Cohort Study. Res Q Exerc Sport. 88(4):401-407.
Super CM. 1976. Environmental effects on motor development: the case of “African infant precocity”. Dev Med Child Neurol. 18(5):561-7.
vam Hof P, Kamp J, Savelsbergh GJP. 2002. The relation of unimanual and bimanual reaching to crossing the midline. Child Dev. 73:1353–1362.
van Roon D, van der Kamp J, Steenbergen B 2003. Constraints in children’s learning to use spoons. In: Savelsbergh G, Davids K, van der Kamp J, Bennett SJ, eds. Development of Movement Co-ordination in Children: Applications in the Fields of Ergonomics, Health Sciences and Sport. Routledge, London: 75-93.
WHO Multicentre Growth Reference Study Group. 2006a. Assessment of sex differences and heterogeneity in motor milestone attainment among populations in the WHO Multicentre Growth Reference Study. Acta Paediatr Suppl. 450:66-75.
WHO Multicentre Growth Reference Study Group. 2006b. WHO Motor Development Study: windows of achievement for six gross motor development milestones. Acta Paediatr Suppl. 450:86-95.
Zelazo PR 1983. The development of walking: New findings and old solutions. Journal of Motor Behavior 15: 99-137.
Zubler JM, Wiggins LD, Macias MM, Whitaker TM, Shaw JS, Squires JK, Pajek JA, Wolf RB, Slaughter KS, Broughton AS, Gerndt KL, Mlodoch BJ, Lipkin PH. 2022. Evidence-Informed Milestones for Developmental Surveillance Tools. Pediatrics. 149(3):e2021052138.
Image credits for “Motor milestones”
image of baby starting to roll over by istock / Gwill
image of baby holding and using spoon by istock/ PeopleImages
charts (adapted from WHO 2006) copyright Parenting Science
content last modified 10/2023
https://www.youtube.com/@OyuncuCrazy?sub_confirmation=1 chi's sweet home,chi cat cartoon,chi the cat,chi the cat song,chi cat ...
Read moreStarting homeschooling can feel very daunting but I hope tht in this video sharing what we do in our home...
Read moreIt’s the most wonderful time of year friends! Time for a new cross-stitch pattern release! This year I’m excited to...
Read moreThree years ago, I watched my son fall in love for the first time. I could tell something was going...
Read moreIntroductory video For Free Education Of English language With Tips And Tricks For Grammatical Items and topics of English ...
Read more